#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

Effects of experimentally induced fatigue on healthy older adults’ gait: A systematic review


Authors: Paulo Cezar Rocha dos Santos aff001;  Fabio Augusto Barbieri aff003;  Inge Zijdewind aff004;  Lilian Teresa Bucken Gobbi aff002;  Claudine Lamoth aff001;  Tibor Hortobágyi aff001
Authors place of work: Center for Human Movement Sciences, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands aff001;  Posture and Gait Studies Laboratory (LEPLO), Graduate Program in Movement Sciences, Institute of Biosciences, São Paulo State University (UNESP), Rio Claro, Brazil aff002;  Human Movement Research Laboratory (MOVI-LAB), Graduate Program in Movement Sciences, Department of Physical Education, São Paulo State University (UNESP), Bauru, Brazil aff003;  Department of Biomedical Sciences of Cells and Systems, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands aff004
Published in the journal: PLoS ONE 14(12)
Category: Research Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0226939

Summary

Introduction

While fatigue is ubiquitous in old age and visibly interferes with mobility, studies have not yet examined the effects of self-reported fatigue on healthy older adults’ gait. As a model that simulates this daily phenomenon, we systematically reviewed eleven studies that compared the effects of experimentally induced muscle and mental performance fatigability on gait kinematics, variability, kinetics, and muscle activity in healthy older adults.

Methods

We searched for studies in databases (PubMed and Web of Science) using Fatigue, Gait, and Clinical conditions as the main terms and extracted the data only from studies that experimentally induced fatigue by sustained muscle or mental activities in healthy older adults.

Results

Eleven studies were included. After muscle performance fatigability, six of nine studies observed increases in stride length, width, gait velocity (Effect Size [ES] range: 0.30 to 1.22), inter-stride trunk acceleration variability (ES: 2.06), and ankle muscle coactivation during gait (ES: 0.59, n = 1 study). After sustained mental activity, the coefficient of variation of stride outcomes increased (ES: 0.59 to 0.67, n = 1 study) during dual-task but not single-task walking.

Conclusion

Muscle performance fatigability affects spatial and temporal features of gait and, mainly, inter-stride trunk acceleration variability. In contrast, sustained mental activity tends only to affect step variability during dual tasking. A critical and immediate step for future studies is to determine the effects of self-reported fatigue on gait biomechanics and variability in healthy older adults to verify the viability of experimentally induced fatigue as a model for the study of gait adaptability in old age.

Keywords:

elderly – Acceleration – kinematics – Gait analysis – Walking – Knees – Ankles – Fatigue

Introduction

Population studies and primary care data show that ~46% of older adults complain about being tired [13]. Tiredness is the sensation of exhaustion, a reduction of physical and mental energy, and a diminished interest in the surrounding world. Prolonged physical or mental exertion can reduce motor performance (performance fatigability) [46] or reduce the capacity to allocate cognitive resources to perform a task [7] and increase self-reported fatigue (perceived fatigability) [8]. Performing a low-force activity for a prolonged period, such as a long high-paced walk, can lead to a sensation of muscle performance fatigability. Performing a motor task at a high percentage of the available maximal mechanical output, i.e., at a high relative effort, can also lead to muscle performance fatigability, a state that is associated with reduced contractile force and a sub-optimal neural activation of muscles [6,8,9]. The decline in force due to sustained muscle effort can interfere with the quality of motor acts such as carrying an object, maintaining bodily postures, and gait [1012].

While prolonged low force and short-term high force motor acts can directly reduce motor performance due to impairment in force and muscle activation, demanding mental activities can also create a psychobiological state characterized by a perception of tiredness and a lack of motivation [8,13,14]. Sustaining attention or a mental effort for a prolonged period puts older adults in a fatigued mental state [7,15,16] that slows cognitive processes often quantified by slowed reaction times [13,14,17]. Sustained mental activity is also associated with alteration of cortical brain areas and decreases in neurotransmitter levels [14,18]. Such modifications may impair top-down cognitive control and the execution of motor tasks indirectly, even in the absence of demonstrable muscle weakness [19,20]. Sustained mental activities can also decrease parasympathetic and increase sympathetic activity, reducing motivation and pre-frontal brain activation [7,20].

While both fatigue types are prevalent in old age and visibly interfere with gait, studies have not yet examined the effects of trait of fatigue on healthy older adults’ gait. To minimize interference and maintain gait quality, older adults are expected to adopt strategies that help to compensate for the mal-effects of fatigue on gait. The unanswered question is whether and how those healthy older adults who report no trait of fatigue can adapt their gait when either kind of fatigue is induced by experimental protocols in a laboratory environment. Such paradigms are thought to simulate performance or perceived fatigued states often reported by older adults. It is important because the after-effects of sustained activities can destabilize gait and posture, increasing the risks for slips, trips, and falls [2124]. The picture emerging from the systematically not yet reviewed studies is that fatigue-free healthy older adults are able somehow to adjust their gait kinematics, kinetics, variability, and muscle activation to states created by performance or perceived fatigue induced in a laboratory environment [2527]. It seems likely that experimentally induced muscle fatigability by prolonged physical activity affects the generation of mechanical work and power at the ankle, knee, and hip joints during gait [28]. Such changes are reasonable because the cellular mechanisms of fatigue impair voluntary force generation and the neural drive of muscles [4,6,29] that generate torques and powers during gait. Specifically, it is likely that older adults would in compensation for the force loss increase stride width and muscle activity to increase gait stability [25]. Subtler mechanisms could involve increases in the activity of antagonist muscles and distribute effort by recruiting less affected muscles at adjacent joints [28,30,31]. Concerning mental fatigability, we expect that interference with attention, arousal, executive function, mood, and motivation would primarily affect gait variability [32,33]. Indeed, brain areas underlying these cognitive functions are also active during imagined walking [34] and are related to temporal step outcomes and gait variability [3537]. We thus hypothesized that gait adaptations might be fatigue-type specific. The purpose of this paper was to systematically review studies that compared the effects of experimentally induced muscle and mental performance fatigability on gait kinematics, variability, kinetics, and muscle activity in healthy older adults. A comprehensive review of these adaptations is timely and needed because it would increase our understanding of how old age affects the capacity to adapt gait to sustained muscle or mental activities.

Methods

We performed a computerized systematic literature search, following PRISMA (S1 Checklist) and Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews guidelines [38,39], in PubMed and Web of Science for the period between January 1987 to August 2019 (last 30 years from the beginning of the search (2017) and updated for the 2 following years). The search consisted of four terms: Term 1 was the population by using the keywords ‘old’, ‘elderly’ and ‘adults’; Term 2 was the intervention ‘Fatigue’ probed with the keywords ‘fatigue’, ‘fatigability’, ‘tiredness’, and its variants (e.g., mental fatigue, physical fatigue, motor fatigue, cognitive fatigue, performance fatigability, and perceived fatigability). Term 3 was the outcomes ‘Gait’ and ‘Walking’ with the outcomes of gait adaptability concerning gait biomechanics, kinetics, kinematics, muscle activity, spatial-temporal parameters, inverse dynamics, gait stability, and gait variability. Term 4 included the exclusion criteria and clinical conditions, such as neurological and orthopaedical diseases. Although the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews suggest that the ‘NOT’ operator should be avoided as exclusion where possible [39], in our case, exclusion terms were necessary as a search strategy to remove from the initial screening the substantial number of papers in diseased populations. Filters were set to include English language (S1 Table). The PubMed syntax was adapted to the Web of Science search. We also identified studies missed by the search from the list of references of relevant individual papers.

Eligibility, study selection and exclusion criteria

We used the Population, Intervention, Comparison, Outcome, and Study design as the criterion for inclusion of papers in this review [38]. Population: older human adults. Intervention: fatigue induced by prolonged physical/muscle and mental tasks. Comparison: gait in fatigue and non-fatigued state. Outcomes: gait kinematics (e.g., spatial and temporal stride parameters, joint angle, joint angular, acceleration outcomes), kinetics (e.g., force outcomes as momentum, work and power, ground reaction force), electromyography (e.g., amplitude and temporal parameters used to assess muscle activation). For the analysis of gait variability and stability, we considered the standard deviation, coefficient of variation, and measures of variability regarding gait dynamics, such as RMS, sample and multi-scale entropy methods, detrended fluctuation analysis, and local dynamic stability and margin of stability, respectively. We also considered gait performed under different conditions such as obstructed gait, level surface walking, and treadmill walking. Finally, randomized controlled trials (RCTs), non-randomized controlled trials (nRCTs), and non-randomized non-controlled trials (nRnCTs) were included.

From the initial yield, obtained by combining original articles from electronic databases and targeted searches, titles and abstracts were screened. When a study was potentially eligible and relevant, it was selected for a full-text analysis and then subjected to a quality analysis. Studies that analyzed the effects of fatigue on gait in age groups other than only in older adults were included, but we considered the data only for older adults (over 63 years). When the information was considered insufficient based on title and abstract alone, the full text was analyzed to decide on inclusion.

We had excluded studies that examined running and stair climbing. In addition, studies unrelated to induced fatigue (decline in performance and/or increase in self-reported fatigue) by sustained physical and mental activities or that could not indicate a measurement of induced fatigue, a lack of quantitative gait outcomes and/or a lack of older adults in the sample were excluded at the initial screening of titles and abstracts.

Quality assessment

Two of the authors (PCRS, FAB) screened candidate papers and worked based on a set of guidelines to improve inter-rater reliability. Both authors analyzed the methodological quality of the included studies by using a quality appraisal tool [40]. This appraisal tool relates to the internal and external validity of the measurement and the generalizability of the results. For each question, ‘1’ is rated when the criterion was met, ‘0.5’ when information is lacking detail or clarity, and ‘0’ if the criterion was missing. A higher total score represents a higher quality of the study. In case of discrepancies between the two authors, a third author (TH) was consulted to make a decision about inclusion.

Data extraction and analysis

Two of the authors (PCRS, FAB) extracted the papers, independently, and synthesized data in tables and together, both authors checked the tables. In case of an indecision, a third author (TH) was consulted. The data were coded for: number of participants, age, sex, protocol to induce performance fatigability (sit-to-stand, cognitive task, walking test), measurement of fatigue (decline in performance, increase in self-reported fatigue), gait protocol (treadmill, level walking, walking with obstacle crossing), and gait outcomes (kinematic and kinetic data, variability, muscle activity). It was not necessary to contact any authors to get information regarding the included papers. We used Cohen’s d to calculate the effect sizes (ES) to quantify whether the magnitude of changes in gait outcomes induced by sustained muscle or mental activity is relevant. ES values of 0.21–0.49 indicate small, 0.50–0.79 indicate medium, and ≥ 0.80 indicate large practical effects [41]. Due to the heterogeneity of the outcomes, lack of consistent results, and the low number of studies that met eligibility criteria, we were unable to perform a meta-analysis.

Results

Study characteristics

The Pubmed and Web of Science searches yielded 1,274 studies and one study was included from the list of reference [42]. After screening for title, abstract and remove the duplicates, 61 studies were selected for analyses, and, after reading the full text, a final sample size of 11 studies was included in the review (Fig 1). The included studies stated the aims sufficiently, gave an appropriate description of the methods, detailed the outcomes clearly, and provided an interpretation of the key findings (S2 Table).

PRISMA flow diagram.
Fig. 1. PRISMA flow diagram.

The current review was based on 249 healthy older adults with a mean age of 71.5 (±4.66) years (Table 1), 92 (37%) females, with normal body composition (body mass index: 26.1±1.94 kg/m2). Two studies did not report the subjects’ sex [23,43] (Table 1).

Tab. 1. Description of the characteristics of the papers selected.
Description of the characteristics of the papers selected.

Effects of fatigue protocols on fatigue outcomes

The studies used heterogeneous protocols to induce a decline in muscle performance, including repeated muscle contractions (n = 7 studies), knee extension/flexions (n = 5), sit-to-stands (n = 4), endurance (n = 3, treadmill and cycling), isokinetic (n = 2, knee and ankle), hip abductions (n = 1), and prolonged mental tasks (n = 1, go/no go task for 90 min) (Tables 1 and 2). Six, three, and two studies indicated the state of fatigue, respectively, as a decline in voluntary force, inability to perform the movement, and movement slowing (Table 1). The reduction in force ranged between ~10 to ~55% and varied between protocols. The sit-to-stand task, for example, reduced voluntary force by ~9 to 13% [25,26]. The isokinetic protocols considered fatigue as reductions to 50% of the initial maximum voluntary torque [27,44]. Unilateral squats performed until exhaustion reduced knee extension force by 17% [45]. Longer reaction time was also observed post vs. pre sustained endurance protocol [21]. Five studies reported an increase in self-reported fatigue (indicated by the rating of perceived exertion and by fatigue perception) (Tables 1 and 2) [21,25,32,43,46]. Two studies [25,46] indicated that older adults reported near maximal perceived exertion (‘very hard’ to ‘maximum exertion’) after repeated muscle contractions. Two studies [21,43] indicated a high rating of self-reported fatigue in response to endurance protocol, scores ranged from ‘hard’ (15) to ‘maximal exertion’ (20) on the Borg scale. One study [32] determined fatigue as a decrease in motivation by 10%, as examined by wakefulness, mood, and arousal dimension of the Multidimensional Mood Questionnaire (ES range: 0.27 to 0.95) and up 2x of increase in fatigue state assessed by Profile of Mood States (ES: 0.92) following a prolonged period of mental activity. One study [23] also indicated an increase (range: 25 to 35 beats per minute) in heart rate (ES: 0.65) after endurance exercise.

Tab. 2. Study characteristics for included studies.
Study characteristics for included studies.

Effects of fatigue protocols on gait

Table 2 shows changes and ESs in gait outcomes after sustained muscle and mental activities. Six studies evaluated outcomes during overground walking with [25,26] or without obstruction [21,25,26,32,44,47] while single- and dual-task walking [32,44]. Five studies assessed the effects of muscle performance fatigability on gait while walking on a motorized treadmill with [27,45] or without a perturbation [23,27,43,45,46].

Muscle performance fatigability affected stride outcomes [23,25,26,4447], gait stability [43], gait variability [44,46,47], and muscle activation [27] during gait. Stride velocity increased by ~10 cm/s (ES = 0.6), stride length by 4.8cm (ES: 0.27) [21,25,44] or by 0.3 units of normalized stride length (ES: 0.63) [23], step width by ~2 cm (ES: 0.80) [25,47], percentage of double support (~2%, ES: 1.22) [23], and a decrease in stride duration by 2ms (ES: 0.42) [25] and standard deviation of stride length by 1 cm (large ES: 0.95) [44] after sustained muscle activity. After muscle performance fatigability, local dynamic stability of 3-D trunk acceleration and symmetry in medial-lateral direction of trunk acceleration decreased by 0.1 max LyE (ES: 0.73) [43] and by 22 in harmonic rate (ES: 0.49) [46], respectively and the anteroposterior and vertical inter-stride trunk acceleration variability increased by 8% and 11% (ES: 0.8 and 2.06), respectively [47]. However, other studies did not indicate effects of muscle performance fatigability on step length (p > 0.05) [26,45,47], and unilateral muscle fatigability protocols did not affect the local dynamic stability during treadmill walking [45,46].

A decline in force induced by sustained muscle activity increased the coactivity between m. soleus and m. tibialis anterior by ~12% (ES: 0.6) and delayed functional reflex activity in the m. tibialis anterior over a 120-ms interval following treadmill decelerations by ~41% (215.7 to 174.7; ES: 0.56) [27]. While walking on an obstacle course, muscle performance fatigability reduced step duration by 5ms (ES: 0.45) and increased step velocity by 6 cm/s (ES: 0.31), step width by 1 cm (ES: 0.20), toe clearance of trailing limb to obstacle by 1 cm (ES: 0.10) [25], and the vertical loading by 4.3 N kg-1 m-1 (ES: 0.27; all p < 0.05) [26]. These results suggest that muscle performance fatigability induced adaptations in the mean and variability of spatial-temporal stride parameters during overground level walking and obstacle negotiation and increased the coactivation and delayed functional muscle reflex during treadmill walking decelerations.

Reduced mental performance was associated with an increased coefficient of variation of gait velocity from ~6% to 11%, stride length from ~4% to 7%, stance time from ~7% to 13%, double support time from ~7% to 16% (ES: 0.50 to 0.68) and swing time by from ~9% to 14% (ES: 0.41, all p < 0.05) during level walking in dual- but not in single-task condition [32].

Discussion

We systematically reviewed studies that compared the effects of experimentally induced muscle and mental performance fatigability on gait kinematics, variability, kinetics, and muscle activity in healthy older adults. Muscle performance fatigability affects spatial and temporal features of gait and, mainly, inter-stride trunk acceleration variability. In contrast, sustained mental activity tends only to affect step variability during dual-tasking. The evidence supports the hypothesis that healthy older adults adapt spatial-temporal features of gait in a fatigue-type specific manner. We discuss these findings with a perspective on whether experimentally induced fatigue is a viable model for the study of gait adaptability in old age.

Muscle fatigue protocols were effective and induced sizable reductions in voluntary force (ES range: 0.30 to 1.32), an accepted marker of performance fatigability [8]. However, the protocols varied widely and included: 1) Repetitive muscle contractions of knee and ankle extensors with different instructions; 2) The STS task performed rapidly or at a fixed speed, and 3) Endurance tasks involving rapid walking for six minutes, incline walking on a treadmill, or incremental cycle-ergometer tests (Table 2). This large variation in methods inducing fatigue is one source of the inconsistent effects on gait because cyclical lower extremity tasks could, in fact, entrain rather than perturb gait, diminishing the interference effects and the need for participants to invoke adaptations in their walking pattern.

It is however curious that even when participants performed ~70 knee extensions or ankle plantarflexions at a maximal effort and the MVC in decreased by 50% (ES: ~1.3) [27,44], changes in spatiotemporal gait variables were moderate but in the unexpected direction (ES: 0.47 to 0.58, Table 2). Indeed, stride length (~4%), gait speed (~10%), and step width (~11%) tended to increase and stride duration (~4%) tended to decrease (ES: 0.4 to 0.8) [21,25,44]. It seems that gait has actually become more dynamic. The step and speed changes might reflect adaptations to the marked increase in trunk acceleration and variability in the vertical and anteroposterior directions (ES: 0.80 to 2.06, Table 2) [47].

Why did performance fatigability not elicit larger changes in gait and necessitate more substantive adaptive responses to the perturbations? One possibility is that torque and power demands during gait were still below the levels of joint torques and powers fatigued muscles could produce [48]. It was also reported that participants could compensate by more strongly activating muscles that were less or not affected by the task [28]. Whether gait is tested overground or on a treadmill affected the results, as studies that reported small fatigue effects on spatial-temporal parameters tested gait using an overground protocol [21,25,44,47] but those that found no effects used a treadmill [45,46]. Walking on a treadmill at a set speed makes gait kinematically uniform and minimizes the potential for adaptations to occur [49,50], especially in step variability [51,52]. This argument is borne out by a lack of fatigue effects on gait when participants were tested on treadmill [45,46] compared with the small but meaningful decreases in the autocorrelation and increases in variability of ML trunk acceleration during overground gait [47]. The use of unilateral fatigue protocols did induce some gait asymmetry but left all other gait outcomes virtually unaffected in older adults [46]. Finally, increases in gait velocity and step length after a fatigue protocol suggest that a warm-up instead of an interference effect might have occurred. However, we need to consider even these small changes in gait with caution because a number of studies reported no changes in gait metrics after a variety of muscle fatigue protocols, making all of the data combined inconsistent [26,45,46].

Muscle performance fatigability can modify muscle activation in single joint tasks and also during gait. For example, decreases in level of force delayed muscle activation onset in older adults while rising from a chair [31]. After ankle muscle fatigability, coactivation of agonist and antagonist ankle muscles increased by ~12% during gait and there was 41% delay in a functional reflex when older adults were prompted to respond to gait perturbations [27]. It is speculated that sustained muscle activity-related increase in coactivation during gait [53,54] reflects changes in the afferent feedback [29,55]. However, such an interpretation is complicated by a coupled increase in plantarflexion angular velocity and increase in coactivation of the soleus and tibialis anterior muscles during gait, a counterintuitive outcome because coactivation would tend to stiffen instead accelerate joint motion. While suggested [28], we found no direct evidence for activation substitution, i.e., reduction in muscle activation of the fatigued muscle group being compensated by increases in activation of muscles at adjacent joints. Together, the evidence is scant that there is an age- and perturbation-specific adaptation in muscle activation in response to fatigue perturbations.

Performing a mental task for a prolonged period increased gait variability only during dual-task gait [32]. This limited effect is in line with the hypothesis emerging from imaging studies suggesting the involvement of complementary brain areas in gait, attention, and executive function while walking and performing a cognitive task at the same time [36,56]. Accordingly, sustained mental activity affects cognitive functions known to be involved in gait control, resulting in an interference with gait automaticity. This interference increases step variability. In single-task conditions, the interference created by the sustained mental activity may not be large enough, producing no measurable effects on any of the gait outcomes reviewed here.

While there has been a concerted effort to use fatigue as a perturbation model (Tables 1 and 2), its viability to study the effects of age on gait adaptability remains unclear. When combined with data from young individuals, the reviewed data revealed a lack of age effect, suggesting that the nature, magnitude, and focality of the perturbations lacks specificity to age and gait. Indeed, fatigue-induced changes in gait were quantitatively similar in healthy younger and older adults and also similar in healthy older adults and Parkinsonian patients [25,28,57]. The original intent of these studies was to make healthy, fatigue-free older adults fatigued to simulate the fatigued state. However, it is unclear if the experimentally induced fatigue state and the fatigue state de novo present in older people are qualitatively and quantitatively similar. It seems that when muscle fatigue is induced with repetitive single-joint muscle contractions such as knee extension-flexion, the ensuing fatigue is predominantly a localized force impairment while the fatigue state in older adults is the result of a combination of impaired physiology, reduced homeostasis, a bias in effort perception, and altered cognitive function. When however, a multi-joint protocol is used (i.e., six-minute walk test), any adaptation in gait after the task is the result of a combined physiological and cognitive (behavioral) effect.

Such limitations and the diversity in fatigue protocols shape the implementation of this perturbation model in the future. The viability of the model will increase if studies move from its descriptive application to hypothesis-driven designs. There is a need to determine the effects of muscle performance fatigability on motor outcomes that are specific and also not specific to the fatigue task, an approach that would improve experimental control and the validity of conclusions. Future studies should also evaluate cognitive outcomes because the adaptive processes may not be confined to motor (gait) function alone. Therefore, future studies should include motor-cognitive dual-task assessments when probing age-differences in adaptations to fatigue. There is a strong need for studies examining the effects of prolonged mental tasks on gait biomechanics and variability. Such studies should set fatigability and gait as the main outcomes in older adults [58]. Such an approach would strengthen our understanding of the role cognition plays in gait control. Perhaps the most critical gap in knowledge is related to a lack of studies comparing gait outcomes in older adults with and without self-reported fatigue. Only after such studies could we meaningfully interpret gait adaptations in healthy older adults after experimentally induced muscle or mental fatigue.

In conclusion, muscle performance fatigability affects spatial and temporal features of gait and, mainly, inter-stride trunk acceleration variability. In contrast, sustained mental activity tends only to affect step variability during dual-tasking. A critical and immediate step for future studies is to determine the effects of self-reported fatigue on gait biomechanics and variability in healthy older adults to verify the viability of experimentally induced fatigue as a model for the study of gait adaptability in old age.

Supporting information

S1 Checklist [doc]
Prisma checklist.

S1 Table [docx]
Search terms.

S2 Table [docx]
Methodological quality appraisal results.


Zdroje

1. Alexander NB, Taffet GE, Horne FM, Eldadah BA, Ferrucci L, Nayfield S, et al. Bedside-to-Bench conference: research agenda for idiopathic fatigue and aging. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2010;58: 967–975. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.02811.x 20722821

2. Lewis G, Wessely S. The epidemiology of fatigue: more questions than answers. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1992;46: 92–97. doi: 10.1136/jech.46.2.92 1583440

3. Liao S, Ferrell BA. Fatigue in an older population. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000;48: 426–30. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10798471 doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2000.tb04702.x 10798471

4. Stackhouse SK, Stevens JE, Lee SC, Pearce KM, Snyder-Mackler L, Binder-Macleod SA. Maximum voluntary activation in nonfatigued and fatigued muscle of young and elderly individuals. Phys Ther. 2001;81: 1102–9. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11319935 11319935

5. Rannou F, Nybo L, Andersen JE, Nordsborg NB. Monitoring Muscle Fatigue Progression during Dynamic Exercise. Med Sci Sport Exerc. 2019; 1. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001921 30741747

6. Gandevia SC. Spinal and supraspinal factors in human muscle fatigue. Physiol Rev. 2001;81: 1725–1789. doi: 10.1152/physrev.2001.81.4.1725 11581501

7. Boksem MAS, Meijman TF, Lorist MM. Effects of mental fatigue on attention: An ERP study. Cogn Brain Res. 2005;25: 107–116. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.011 15913965

8. Enoka RM, Duchateau J. Translating Fatigue to Human Performance. Med Sci Sport Exerc. 2016;48: 2228–2238. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000929 27015386

9. Davis JM. Central and peripheral factors in fatigue. J Sports Sci. Taylor & Francis Group; 1995;13: S49–S53. doi: 10.1080/02640419508732277 8897320

10. Grenier JG, Millet GY, Peyrot N, Samozino P, Oullion R, Messonnier L, et al. Effects of extreme-duration heavy load carriage on neuromuscular function and locomotion: a military-based study. Lucia A, editor. PLoS One. 2012;7: e43586. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043586 22927995

11. Salavati M, Moghadam M, Ebrahimi I, Arab AM. Changes in postural stability with fatigue of lower extremity frontal and sagittal plane movers. Gait Posture. 2007;26: 214–8. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.09.001 17049237

12. Paillard T. Effects of general and local fatigue on postural control: A review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. Pergamon; 2012;36: 162–176. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.05.009 21645543

13. Boksem MAS, Tops M. Mental fatigue: Costs and benefits. Brain Res Rev. 2008;59: 125–139. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.07.001 18652844

14. Lorist MM, Boksem MAS, Ridderinkhof KR. Impaired cognitive control and reduced cingulate activity during mental fatigue. Cogn Brain Res. 2005;24: 199–205. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.018 15993758

15. Wascher E, Rasch B, Sänger J, Hoffmann S, Schneider D, Rinkenauer G, et al. Frontal theta activity reflects distinct aspects of mental fatigue. Biol Psychol. 2014;96: 57–65. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.11.010 24309160

16. de Jong M, Jolij J, Pimenta A, Lorist MM. Age Modulates the Effects of Mental Fatigue on Typewriting. Front Psychol. 2018;9: 1113. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01113 30042705

17. Van Cutsem J, Marcora S, De Pauw K, Bailey S, Meeusen R, Roelands B. The Effects of Mental Fatigue on Physical Performance: A Systematic Review. Sport Med. 2017;47: 1569–1588. doi: 10.1007/s40279-016-0672-0 28044281

18. Pires FO, Silva-Júnior FL, Brietzke C, Franco-Alvarenga PE, Pinheiro FA, de França NM, et al. Mental Fatigue Alters Cortical Activation and Psychological Responses, Impairing Performance in a Distance-Based Cycling Trial. Front Physiol. Frontiers Media SA; 2018;9: 227. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.00227 29615923

19. Lorist MM. Impact of top-down control during mental fatigue. Brain Res. 2008;1232: 113–123. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.07.053 18687317

20. Mizuno K, Tanaka M, Yamaguti K, Kajimoto O, Kuratsune H, Watanabe Y. Mental fatigue caused by prolonged cognitive load associated with sympathetic hyperactivity. Behav Brain Funct. BioMed Central; 2011;7: 17. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-7-17 21605411

21. Morrison S, Colberg SR, Parson HK, Neumann S, Handel R, Vinik EJ, et al. Walking-Induced Fatigue Leads to Increased Falls Risk in Older Adults. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2016;17: 402–409. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2015.12.013 26825684

22. Lew FL, Qu X. Effects of mental fatigue on biomechanics of slips. Ergonomics. 2014;57: 1927–32. doi: 10.1080/00140139.2014.937771 25017252

23. Nagano H, James L, Sparrow WA, Begg RK. Effects of walking-induced fatigue on gait function and tripping risks in older adults. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2014;11: 155. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-11-155 25399324

24. Parijat P, Lockhart TE. Effects of quadriceps fatigue on the biomechanics of gait and slip propensity. Gait Posture. 2008;28: 568–73. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.04.001 18514522

25. Barbieri FA, dos Santos PCR, Simieli L, Orcioli-Silva D, Van Dieën JH, Gobbi LTB. Interactions of age and leg muscle fatigue on unobstructed walking and obstacle crossing. Gait Posture. Elsevier; 2014;39: 985–990. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2013.12.021 24440157

26. Hatton AL, Menant JC, Lord SR, Lo JCM, Sturnieks DL. The effect of lower limb muscle fatigue on obstacle negotiation during walking in older adults. Gait Posture. 2013;37: 506–10. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.09.004 23021990

27. Granacher U, Gruber M, Förderer D, Strass D, Gollhofer A. Effects of ankle fatigue on functional reflex activity during gait perturbations in young and elderly men. Gait Posture. 2010;32: 107–12. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.03.016 20434345

28. Barbieri FA, Gobbi LTB, Lee YJ, Pijnappels M, van Dieën JH. Effect of triceps surae and quadriceps muscle fatigue on the mechanics of landing in stepping down in ongoing gait. Ergonomics. 2014;57: 934–942. doi: 10.1080/00140139.2014.903302 24697241

29. Taylor JL, Amann M, Duchateau J, Meeusen R, Rice CL. Neural Contributions to Muscle Fatigue: From the Brain to the Muscle and Back Again. Med Sci Sports Exerc. NIH Public Access; 2016;48: 2294–2306. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000923 27003703

30. Barbieri FA, Beretta SS, Pereira VAI, Simieli L, Orcioli-Silva D, dos Santos PCR, et al. Recovery of gait after quadriceps muscle fatigue. Gait Posture. 2016;43: 270–4. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2015.10.015 26531768

31. Bryanton MA, Bilodeau M. The influence of knee extensor fatigue on lower extremity muscle activity during chair rise in young and older adults. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2019;119: 61–71. doi: 10.1007/s00421-018-3999-4 30317389

32. Behrens M, Mau-Moeller A, Lischke A, Katlun F, Gube M, Zschorlich V, et al. Mental Fatigue Increases Gait Variability During Dual-task Walking in Old Adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2018;73: 792–797. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glx210 29077783

33. Verlinden VJA, van der Geest JN, Hofman A, Ikram MA. Cognition and gait show a distinct pattern of association in the general population. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2014;10: 328–335. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2013.03.009 23849591

34. Iseki K, Hanakawa T, Shinozaki J, Nankaku M, Fukuyama H. Neural mechanisms involved in mental imagery and observation of gait. Neuroimage. 2008;41: 1021–1031. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.010 18450480

35. Zwergal A, Linn J, Xiong G, Brandt T, Strupp M, Jahn K. Aging of human supraspinal locomotor and postural control in fMRI. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33: 1073–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.09.022 21051105

36. Lo O-Y, Halko MA, Zhou J, Harrison R, Lipsitz LA, Manor B. Gait Speed and Gait Variability Are Associated with Different Functional Brain Networks. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017;9: 390. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00390 29249961

37. Studenski S, Perera S, Patel K, Rosano C, Faulkner K, Inzitari M, et al. Gait speed and survival in older adults. JAMA. NIH Public Access; 2011;305: 50–8. doi: 10.1001/jama.2010.1923 21205966

38. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, PRISMA Group. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: The PRISMA Statement. J Clin Epidemiol. 2009;62: 1006–1012. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2009.06.005 19631508

39. Higgins J, Green S. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. 7th ed. Higgins J, Green S, editors. West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons Ltd; 2017.

40. Galna B, Peters A, Murphy AT, Morris ME. Obstacle crossing deficits in older adults: A systematic review. Gait Posture. Elsevier; 2009;30: 270–275. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.05.022 19625191

41. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd. Hillsdale (NJ): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.

42. Da Rocha ES, Kunzler MR, Bobbert MF, Duysens J, Carpes FP. 30 min of treadmill walking at self-selected speed does not increase gait variability in independent elderly. J Sports Sci. 2018;36: 1305–1311. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2017.1375139 28869740

43. Hamacher D, Törpel A, Hamacher D, Schega L. The effect of physical exhaustion on gait stability in young and older individuals. Gait Posture. 2016;48: 137–139. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.05.007 27239774

44. Granacher U, Wolf I, Wehrle A, Bridenbaugh S, Kressig RW. Effects of muscle fatigue on gait characteristics under single and dual-task conditions in young and older adults. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2010;7: 56. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-7-56 21062458

45. Toebes MJP, Hoozemans MJM, Dekker J, van Dieën JH. Effects of unilateral leg muscle fatigue on balance control in perturbed and unperturbed gait in healthy elderly. Gait Posture. 2014;40: 215–219. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2014.03.194 24768117

46. Arvin M, Hoozemans MJM, Burger BJ, Rispens SM, Verschueren SMP, van Dieën JH, et al. Effects of hip abductor muscle fatigue on gait control and hip position sense in healthy older adults. Gait Posture. 2015;42: 545–549. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2015.08.011 26386676

47. Helbostad JL, Leirfall S, Moe-Nilssen R, Sletvold O. Physical fatigue affects gait characteristics in older persons. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2007;62: 1010–5. doi: 10.1093/gerona/62.9.1010 17895440

48. Hunt MA, Hatfield GL. Ankle and knee biomechanics during normal walking following ankle plantarflexor fatigue. J Electromyogr Kinesiol. 2017;35: 24–29. doi: 10.1016/j.jelekin.2017.05.007 28587934

49. Malatesta D, Canepa M, Menendez Fernandez A. The effect of treadmill and overground walking on preferred walking speed and gait kinematics in healthy, physically active older adults. Eur J Appl Physiol. Springer Berlin Heidelberg; 2017;117: 1833–1843. doi: 10.1007/s00421-017-3672-3 28687953

50. Lee SJ, Hidler J. Biomechanics of overground vs. treadmill walking in healthy individuals. J Appl Physiol. 2008;104: 747–755. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.01380.2006 18048582

51. Hollman JH, Watkins MK, Imhoff AC, Braun CE, Akervik KA, Ness DK. A comparison of variability in spatiotemporal gait parameters between treadmill and overground walking conditions. Gait Posture. Elsevier; 2016;43: 204–209. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2015.09.024 26481257

52. Hanley B, Tucker CB. Gait variability and symmetry remain consistent during high-intensity 10,000 m treadmill running. J Biomech. 2018;79: 129–134. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2018.08.008 30126720

53. Kellis E, Kellis S. Effects of agonist and antagonist muscle fatigue on muscle coactivation around the knee in pubertal boys. J Electromyogr Kinesiol. 2001;11: 307–318. doi: 10.1016/s1050-6411(01)00014-1 11595550

54. Arellano CJ, Caha D, Hennessey JE, Amiridis IG, Baudry S, Enoka RM. Fatigue-induced adjustment in antagonist coactivation by old adults during a steadiness task. J Appl Physiol. American Physiological Society; 2016;120: 1039–46. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00908.2015 26846553

55. Hortobagyi T, Tracy J, Hamilton G, Lambert J. Fatigue Effects on Muscle Excitability. Int J Sports Med. 1996;17: 409–414. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-972870 8884414

56. Allali G, Montembeault M, Brambati SM, Bherer L, Blumen HM, Launay CP, et al. Brain Structure Covariance Associated with Gait Control in Aging. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2018; doi: 10.1093/gerona/gly123 29846517

57. Santos PCR, Gobbi LTB, Orcioli-Silva D, Simieli L, van Dieën JH, Barbieri FA. Effects of leg muscle fatigue on gait in patients with Parkinson’s disease and controls with high and low levels of daily physical activity. Gait Posture. 2016;47: 86–91. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.04.002 27264409

58. Tieland M, Trouwborst I, Clark BC. Skeletal muscle performance and ageing. J Cachexia Sarcopenia Muscle. Springer Nature; 2018;9: 3–19. doi: 10.1002/jcsm.12238 29151281


Článok vyšiel v časopise

PLOS One


2019 Číslo 12
Najčítanejšie tento týždeň
Najčítanejšie v tomto čísle
Kurzy

Zvýšte si kvalifikáciu online z pohodlia domova

Získaná hemofilie - Povědomí o nemoci a její diagnostika
nový kurz

Eozinofilní granulomatóza s polyangiitidou
Autori: doc. MUDr. Martina Doubková, Ph.D.

Všetky kurzy
Prihlásenie
Zabudnuté heslo

Zadajte e-mailovú adresu, s ktorou ste vytvárali účet. Budú Vám na ňu zasielané informácie k nastaveniu nového hesla.

Prihlásenie

Nemáte účet?  Registrujte sa

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#