#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

Relatives’ attachment anxiety mediates the association between perceived loss and expressed emotion in early psychosis


Authors: Lídia Hinojosa-Marqués aff001;  Tecelli Domínguez-Martínez aff002;  Tamara Sheinbaum aff003;  Paula Cristóbal-Narváez aff004;  Thomas R. Kwapil aff005;  Neus Barrantes-Vidal aff001
Authors place of work: Departament de Psicologia Clínica i de la Salut, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain aff001;  Centro de Investigación en Salud Mental Global, Dirección de Investigaciones Epidemiológicas y Psicosociales, Instituto Nacional de Psiquiatría “Ramón de la Fuente Muñiz”, Mexico City, Mexico aff002;  Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, United States of America aff003;  Unitat de Recerca, Docència i Innovació, Parc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu, San Boi de Llobregat, Barcelona, Spain aff004;  Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, Illinois, United States of America aff005;  Sant Pere Claver- Fundació Sanitària, Barcelona, Spain aff006;  Centre for Biomedical Research Network on Mental Health (CIBERSAM), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Barcelona, Spain aff007
Published in the journal: PLoS ONE 14(10)
Category: Research Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223425

Summary

A common reaction experienced by family members of patients with psychosis is grief for the loss of their healthy relative. Importantly, high levels of perceived loss have been related to the manifestation of high expressed emotion (EE), which includes the negative attitudes expressed by relatives toward an ill family member. However, the mechanisms underlying the relationship between relatives’ perceived loss and EE attitudes in the early stages of psychosis are still not fully understood. In this regard, attachment theory has been suggested as a useful framework for understanding this link. The current study aimed to examine: (1) whether relatives’ perceived loss was associated with relatives’ EE dimensions (i.e., criticism and emotional over-involvement (EOI)), and (2) whether such associations were mediated by relatives’ attachment dimensions (i.e., anxiety and avoidance). Seventy-eight relatives of patients with early psychosis completed the Mental Illness Version of the Texas Inventory of Grief for the assessment of loss reactions. Attachment dimensions and EE attitudes were assessed by the Psychosis Attachment Measure and the Family Questionnaire, respectively. Findings indicated that relatives’ perceived loss was associated with EE dimensions. Relatives’ attachment anxiety, but not avoidance, mediated the relationship of perceived loss with both criticism and EOI. Findings highlight the importance of examining the role of relatives’ attachment characteristics for understanding how perceptions of loss might impact the manifestation of EE attitudes in the early stages of psychosis. Family interventions aimed at assisting relatives to improve their management of negative emotional reactions to loss are fundamental to prevent impairing loss reactions and the entrenchment of high-EE attitudes.

Keywords:

Psychological stress – Mental health and psychiatry – emotions – anxiety – memory – Questionnaires – Psychoses – Psychometrics

Introduction

A prominent reaction described by family members of patients with psychosis is grief [13]. This grief results from a deep sense of loss: loss of the healthy relative, loss of specific hopes and aspirations for the relatives’ future, and loss of the pre-existing relationship between the relative and the patient [46]. Perceived loss has been found to have negative implications for relatives’ physical and psychological health, and also affects interactions with their ill family member [7, 8]. However, grief is not necessarily a pathological phenomenon, but rather a natural process of adapting to or accommodating a changed reality [9].

Relatives’ perceived loss has been mostly examined in schizophrenia caregivers [1012]. However, the few studies examining relatives’ loss reactions in the early stages of psychosis indicated that high levels of perceived loss are already present in early phases of the illness [1315]. Moreover, high levels of loss appraisals are related to the manifestation of high expressed emotion (EE) attitudes (i.e., criticism and emotional over-involvement (EOI); [1316]). Patterson et al. [14, 15] found that high levels of perceived loss were related to high EOI attitudes, whereas low levels of perceived loss were linked to high criticism. It seems that relatives’ perception of loss could motivate high EOI attitudes with the aim of reestablishing what has been lost [14, 15]. However, coercive criticism has also been theoretically interpreted as a coping mechanism to deal with the pain of loss [17]. As argued by Patterson et al. [14, 15], this interpretation is consistent with attachment theory, which states that coercive angry or critical attitudes are a natural response to any perceived loss, with the goal to bring the person “back into line” through an activation of attachment responses [1820]. Since the perception of loss may be a major driver of high EE attitudes [21, 22], it would be relevant to identify the mediating mechanisms by which perceived loss influences the manifestation of EE in early psychosis relatives. In this regard, attachment theory has been highlighted as a useful framework for understanding how loss appraisals may contribute to the development of EE [14, 15, 23]. Specifically, relatives’ attachment styles have received theoretical attention as potential underlying mechanisms in the association between perceived loss and EE attitudes [23].

Attachment theory has become one of the foremost paradigms for understanding the grieving process after the loss of a significant person [2426]. Insecure attachment has been proposed as a major risk factor for complications in adaptation to loss [2728]. Regarding the dimensions underlying attachment style, attachment anxiety and avoidance [2930], it seems that anxious attachment is more consistently associated with elevated grief symptoms than avoidant attachment after the loss of a loved one [3132]. Moreover, anxious/ambivalent attachment has been related to increased parental grief in response to a family member’s mental illness [33].

Attachment theory indicates that threats and stressors to an attachment bond, such as illness or loss, tend to activate the attachment system [3034]. Indeed, persons with high levels of attachment anxiety or avoidance tend to rely on secondary attachment strategies [3536], either hyperactivating or deactivating their attachment system for regulating threat-related distress. Hyperactivating strategies observed in anxious individuals cause them to amplify negative emotional responses to threats. In contrast, avoidant individuals attempt to inhibit emotional reactions with the goal of minimizing activation of the attachment system [30, 34, 37]. Thus, anxiously attached individuals’ tendency to intensify negative emotions would involve a heightened risk of distress after an actual or perceived loss, whereas avoidant individuals’ tendency to suppress negative emotions would promote an absence of conscious grieving [24, 37, 38].

According to Bowlby [20], parents develop their caregiving behaviors in consonance with their attachment systems and associated working models [3942]. For instance, avoidant individuals’ lack of interpersonal engagement and negative working models of others may also translate into a lack of caregiving behaviors, probably driven by deactivating strategies across both attachment and caregiving systems [43, 44]. Conversely, anxious individuals, who adopt a hyperactivating attachment strategy [4345]. show a pattern of hyperactivated caregiving, which is intrusive or overinvolved, poorly timed, and burdensome [30, 34]. The parallelism between these caregiving behaviors and those exhibited by high EE individuals suggests that relatives’ insecure attachment styles may influence their caregiving behaviors, contributing to high EE levels under acute stress [46]. Therefore, insecure attachment has been hypothesized to contribute to the development of critical or EOI parenting styles [47, 48]. Specifically, attachment anxiety has been related to intrusive, overinvolved, and controlling forms of caregiving [42, 44, 49, 50], as well as with the use of authoritarian and dominant behaviors in high anxiety situations [51, 52]. Altogether, this emphasizes attachment theory as a potential framework for understanding how relatives’ attachment history influences the development of EE [53].

The relationship between relatives’ attachment styles and perceived loss in the early stages of psychosis has not yet been explored. Similarly, no studies have directly considered the possible mediating role of insecure attachment styles in the association between relatives’ perceived loss and high EE attitudes. This study addressed these issues in a sample of relatives of early psychosis participants by: 1) examining whether relatives’ perceived loss is associated with EE dimensions (i.e., criticism and EOI); and 2) testing whether such associations are mediated by relatives’ dimensions of attachment (i.e., anxiety and avoidance). Despite the lack of previous studies, it was hypothesized based on the reviewed literature that relatives’ attachment anxiety would play a mediating role between perceived loss and EOI, as well as between perceived loss and criticism.

Materials and methods

Participants

The study included 78 relatives of early psychosis participants, 48 of At-Risk Mental State (ARMS) and 30 of First-Episode (FEP) participants. They were recruited in the Sant Pere Claver-Early Psychosis Program conducted in Barcelona, Spain [54]. Participating relatives were those who had the most regular contact and/or the most significant relationship with the patient. ARMS patients had to meet ARMS criteria as assessed by the Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental States [55] and/or the Schizophrenia Proneness Instrument Adult- Version [56]. FEP patients met DSM-IV-TR criteria [57] for any psychotic disorder or affective disorder with psychotic symptoms as established by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV [58].

All relatives provided written informed consent. The project was developed in accordance with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki). Ethical approval was granted by the Ethics Committee of the Unió Catalana d’Hospitals (Comitè d’Ètica d’Investigació Clínica (CEIC); number 09–40) and by the Ethics Committee of the Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona (Comissió d'Ètica en l'Experimentació Animal i Humana (CEEAH); number 2679).

Measures

Relatives completed the Mental Illness Version of the Texas Inventory of Grief (MIV-TIG) [2] for the assessment of loss reactions. The MIV-TIG is a self-reported questionnaire that contains 24 items that measure relatives’ initial response to the loss of a family member’s mental health and the current feelings about their perceived loss. Responses were rated on a five-point Likert scale from ‘completely true’ to ‘completely false’. The Cronbach’s alpha of the instrument was excellent in this sample (0.95).

Attachment was assessed with the Psychosis Attachment Measure (PAM) [59], a self-reported adult attachment questionnaire referring to self-identified thoughts, feelings and behaviors in interpersonal relationships. The PAM contains 16 items that assess the two dimensions of adult attachment, anxiety and avoidance. Responses are made on a four-point Likert scale ranging from ‘not at all’ to ‘very much’. Reliability for these subscales was acceptable in this sample (Anxiety = 0.71; Avoidance = 0.68).

Finally, relatives completed the Family Questionnaire (FQ) [60], a well-established instrument to measure EE. The FQ consists of 20 items equally distributed into two subscales (EOI and criticism) and scored on a four-point Likert scale ranging from ‘never/very rarely’ to ‘very often’. Reliability for the two subscales in this sample was 0.84 for EOI and 0.87 for criticism.

Statistical analysis

Pearson correlations were calculated to explore the association of relatives’ perceived loss with EOI and criticism, as well as association of these variables with the two attachment dimensions. The effect size of the correlations was interpreted following Cohen’s [61] guidelines (correlations of 0.10 indicate small effect sizes, 0.30 indicate medium effect sizes, and 0.50 indicate large effect sizes). Parallel multiple mediation analyses were performed using PROCESS [62]. These analyses examined the unique mediating effect of attachment anxiety and avoidance on the significant associations found between perceived loss and the EE dimensions. For each model, attachment anxiety and avoidance were entered simultaneously as mediators. The 95% bias-corrected confidence intervals were generated using bootstrapping with 10,000 resamples. Indirect effects were considered significant when the 95% bias-corrected confidence intervals did not include zero.

Results

Most relatives were parents who lived with the patient. Over half of them were females, specifically patients’ mothers (see Table 1 for details about relatives' socio-demographic characteristics). For the sake of completeness, descriptive data for all relatives’ measures are presented in Table 2.

Tab. 1. Descriptive data on socio-demographic characteristics of early psychosis relatives (n = 78).
Descriptive data on socio-demographic characteristics of early psychosis relatives (n = 78).
Tab. 2. Descriptive data on perceived loss, attachment and expressed emotion (N = 78).
Descriptive data on perceived loss, attachment and expressed emotion (N = 78).

Firstly, we examined whether relatives of ARMS and FEP participants differed on levels of perceived loss. No significant differences emerged between them (t(76) = -1.17; p = 0.25; d = 0.27). Consequently, ARMS and FEP relatives were grouped together in the remaining analyses.

Relatives’ perceived loss was significantly related to both EOI (r = 0.49, p <0.001) and criticism (r = 0.27, p<0.05). Following Cohen [61], effect sizes were of a medium and small magnitude, respectively.

As seen in Table 3, relatives’ attachment anxiety showed significant associations with relatives’ perceived loss and both EE dimensions. All effect sizes were of medium magnitude. In contrast, relatives’ attachment avoidance was significantly associated with relatives’ perceived loss but was not associated with the EE dimensions.

Tab. 3. Pearson correlations of attachment dimensions with perceived loss and expressed emotion (N = 78).
Pearson correlations of attachment dimensions with perceived loss and expressed emotion (N = 78).

Table 4 displays the results of the parallel multiple mediation analyses using relatives’ perceived loss as the independent variable. Two models were tested (one for EOI and one for criticism), with the two attachment dimensions entered simultaneously as mediators. The total, direct, and indirect effects are shown in Table 4. The specific indirect effect of attachment anxiety was significant in the models for both EOI and criticism indicating that anxious attachment mediated the association of perceived loss with both EE factors. Note that the direct relationship of perceived loss and criticism no longer remained significant when the indirect pathway through anxious attachment was included. Avoidant attachment was not a significant mediator of the association between relatives’ perceived loss and EE dimensions.

Tab. 4. Mediation analyses examining the indirect effects of perceived loss on EOI and criticism via anxious and avoidant attachment.
Mediation analyses examining the indirect effects of perceived loss on EOI and criticism via anxious and avoidant attachment.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study examining the relationship among relatives' perceived loss, attachment styles, and EE, as well as the mediating role of relatives’ attachment dimensions in the association between perceived loss and EE, in the early stages of psychosis. Results demonstrated that relatives’ perceived loss was associated with EE attitudes as well as with relatives’ attachment anxiety and avoidance. These findings point out the importance of considering the role of relatives’ perceived loss along with other psychological mechanisms for understanding the ontogenesis of EE in the family environment. In addition, the current study indicated that relatives’ attachment anxiety, but not attachment avoidance, mediated the relationship between perceived loss and EE, both for criticism and EOI attitudes. Altogether, these results emphasize the importance of considering the role of loss in the development of EE attitudes and that relatives’ attachment style is relevant to understand how appraisals of loss might impact the formation and/or expression of EOI and criticism in the critical period of the emergence of the disorder.

Consistent with our predictions, relatives’ perceived loss was positively associated with both EOI and criticism. These results are in consonance with theoretical suggestions from the EE literature that relatives’ perception of loss may be a primary driver of high EE attitudes [21, 22]. The positive association between perceived loss and relatives’ EOI attitudes supports previous findings by Patterson et al. [14, 15] in relatives of early psychosis patients. However, in contrast to Patterson et al. [14, 15], the present study also found a positive association between perceived loss and relatives’ criticism. This finding is consistent with attachment theory that indicates that coercive criticism is not an uncommon response to perceived loss as an attempt to restore what has been “lost” through the activation of attachment responses [18, 19, 20].

Anxious attachment in relatives, but not avoidant attachment, mediated the association of the perception of loss with both EOI and criticism. As hypothesized, this differential role of attachment styles might be explained by the specific distress regulation strategies characterizing anxious and avoidant attachment. Anxious individuals respond to appraisals of loss with hyperactivating strategies of the attachment system, which impairs their capacity to regulate negative emotions and thus contributes to an intensification of distress [30, 34, 37]. Moreover, ineffective emotional regulation strategies may make relatives feel overwhelmed by personal distress, which may in turn interfere with the normative functioning of the caregiving behavioral system [30, 34]. Theoretically, the caregiving system is activated by cues generated from care recipients in times of need [20], but it can also be activated by caregivers’ appraisals of danger or threat, which are directly influenced by caregivers’ own attachment system [40, 41]. Specifically, attachment anxiety has been associated with high levels of proximity and compulsive caregiving that are inconsistent and lack sensitivity [42, 63]. Following Canterberry and Gillath [43], this type of caregiving behaviors could reflect the hyperactivation of the caregiving system. Therefore, anxious individuals, who rely on hyperactivating strategies in their attachment system, seem to adopt this strategy with their caregiving system [4345]. Drawing on previous theoretical suggestions based on the interplay between the attachment and caregiving systems [45, 52], our findings suggest that relatives’ perceived loss might trigger the concurrent hyperactivation of the attachment and caregiving systems in anxiously attached relatives. The inability to regulate the negative emotions provoked by perceived loss may lead to excessive ruminations about the loss, amplifying the negative affective/cognitive reactions. This would contribute to the hyperactivation of the caregiving system and lead to EE-like intrusive, overinvolved, or even critical patterns of caregiving, presumably driven by an attempt to restore the pre-illness situation and to mitigate the loss perceived (Fig 1). This explanation is partly consistent with previous suggestions (e.g., [14, 15]), even though the contribution of the caregiving behavioural system as the mechanism through which EE attitudes manifest had not been considered. Specifically, Patterson [23] proposed a theoretical model in which family illness leads to an activation of carers’ and patients’ attachment behaviors. Therefore, relatives increased high EE attitudes should be conceived as an initial normative reaction.

Attachment anxiety as a mechanism linking relatives’ perceived loss and EE.
Fig. 1. Attachment anxiety as a mechanism linking relatives’ perceived loss and EE.
Proposed mediational model of the relationship between relatives’ perceived loss and high EE attitudes (i.e., criticism and EOI) in anxiously attached relatives. Adapted from Patterson (2013).

Previous work has linked attachment anxiety to caregiving behaviors that clearly resemble aspects of EOI behaviors (i.e., overinvolved, intrusive, controlling and/or compulsive forms of caregiving; [42, 44, 49, 50]). This has even led to the interpretation of EOI as a type of anxious attachment/caregiving behavior [64, 65]. In addition, attachment anxiety has also been related to the expression of critical attitudes through the use of authoritarian [52], aggressive [66, 67], dominant [51], and demanding behaviors in high anxiety situations [68]. Therefore, relatives’ insecure attachment could contribute to the development of either critical or EOI parenting styles [47, 48] and these two dimensions of EE could be considered as “special forms of attachment/caregiving” that are likely to lead to dysfunctional patterns of communication and problem solving [65].

Interestingly, an unexpected positive correlation between perceived loss and attachment avoidance was observed. It is well recognized that the tendency to suppress negative emotions encourages an absence of conscious grieving [24, 38, 69]. Nevertheless, some studies have found an association between attachment avoidance and severe grief symptoms after the loss of a loved one [7072]. These findings have been interpreted as indicating that strategies of emotional deactivation might collapse under conditions of high cognitive load produced by highly stressful situations, thereby causing avoidant individuals to be less able to suppress separation thoughts. Therefore, the powerful threat of loss experienced by relatives of early psychosis individuals might weaken the suppression of the negative emotional experience of loss in relatives with avoidant attachment.

Some limitations of the present study must be considered. First, the cross-sectional design limits drawing causal conclusions, which can only be determined by further longitudinal studies. Second, it is important to note that attachment and EE-related constructs are dynamic processes influenced by bidirectional and interactional patterns within familiar relationships [73, 74]. Future studies should consider how the clinical features and attachment styles of care recipients contribute to the manifestation of high-EE attitudes within the family environment. Finally, it should be noted that although the FQ has been recognized as a reliable and valid measure to assess EE [60], it has not yet shown enough predictive validity for relapse in both chronic and early psychosis samples (e.g., [75, 76]). Therefore, further research is required to examine its predictive relevance in the course of the disorder.

In conclusion, this study shows that relatives’ perception of loss is significantly associated with EE-criticism and EE-EOI attitudes in the early stages of coping with psychosis, and that relatives’ attachment anxiety is a mediator of the association between perceived loss and the manifestation of EE attitudes in early psychosis relatives. These results have important clinical applications. Considering the negative implications of perceived loss for relatives’ psychological health and the potential etiological role of perceived loss on the development of high EE attitudes, family interventions should specifically take into consideration the psychological aspects related to loss and the grief process experienced by relatives during the at-risk and onset stages of psychosis. This emphasizes the importance to go beyond educating relatives about psychosis (e.g., [77]) by implementing treatments in which the provision of information is adapted to caregivers’ needs and allows emotional processing [78]. Consequently, this would help relatives to gain a better understanding of their own psychological situation. In addition, these findings highlight the need of tailoring interventions to relatives' attachment needs. This would include assessing relatives' attachment patterns, since the specific subtypes of insecure attachment style will provide guidance to moving forward in the therapeutic process, for example in building the working alliance or setting relevant treatment goals [79]. Early psychosis is a critical period in which relatives’ appraisals and attitudes are forming and might be more malleable; therefore, it is fundamental to support relatives in adjusting to the grief process with the aim of preventing unresolved loss reactions and the entrenchment of high-EE attitudes over time.


Zdroje

1. Davis DJ, Schultz CL. Grief, parenting, and schizophrenia. Soc Sci Med. 1998;46: 369–379. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(97)00167-6 9460818

2. Miller F, Dworkin J, Ward M, Barone D. A preliminary study of unresolved grief in families of seriously mentally ill patients. Hosp Community Psychiatry. 1990;41: 1321–1325. doi: 10.1176/ps.41.12.1321 2276725

3. Osborne J, Coyle A. Can parental responses to adult children with schizophrenia be conceptualized in terms of loss and grief? A case study analysis. Couns Psychol Q. 2002;15: 307–323.

4. Miller F. Grief therapy for relatives of persons with serious mental illness. Psychiatr Serv. 1996;47: 633–637. doi: 10.1176/ps.47.6.633 8726492

5. Parker BA. Living with mental illness: the family as caregiver. J Psychosoc Nurs Ment Heal Serv. 1993;31: 19–21.

6. Young J, Bailey G, Rycroft P. Family grief and mental health: a systemic, contextual and compassionate analysis. Aust N Z J Fam Ther. 2004;24: 188–197.

7. Geraghty K, McCann K, King R, Eichmann K. Sharing the load: parents and carers talk to consumer consultants at a child and youth mental health inpatient unit. Int J Ment Health Nurs. 2011;20: 253–262. doi: 10.1111/j.1447-0349.2011.00730.x 21362120

8. Richardson M, Cobham V, McDermott B, Murray J. Youth mental illness and the family: parents’ loss and grief. J Child Fam Stud. 2013;22: 719–736.

9. MacGregor P. Grief: The unrecognized parental response to mental illness in a child. Soc Work. 1994;39: 160–166. 8153758

10. Jones DW. Families and serious mental illness: working with loss and ambivalence. Br J Soc Work. 2004;34: 961–979.

11. Ozgul S. Parental grief and serious mental illness: a narrative. Aust. N. Z. J. Fam. Ther. 2004; 25: 183–187.

12. Richardson M, Cobham V, Murray J, McDermott B. Parents’ grief in the context of adult child mental illness: a qualitative review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2011;14: 28–43. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0075-y 20645000

13. Mulligan J, Sellwood W, Reid GS, Riddell S, Andy N. Informal caregivers in early psychosis: evaluation of need for psychosocial intervention and unresolved grief. Early Interv Psychiatry. 2013;7: 291–299. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-7893.2012.00369.x 22741743

14. Patterson PJ, Birchwood M, Cochrane R. Preventing the entrenchment of high expressed emotion in first episode psychosis: early developmental attachment pathways. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2000;34 Suppl: S191–S197.

15. Patterson P, Birchwood M, Cochrane R. Expressed emotion as an adaptation to loss: prospective study in first-episode psychosis. Br J Psychiatry. 2005;187 Suppl: S59–S64.

16. Raune D, Kuipers E, Bebbington PE. Expressed emotion at first-episode psychosis: investigating a carer appraisal model. Br J Psychiatry. 2004;184: 321–326. doi: 10.1192/bjp.184.4.321 15056576

17. Stirling J, Tantam D, Thomas P, Newby D, Montague L, Ring N, et al. Expressed emotion and schizophrenia: the ontogeny of EE during an 18-month follow-up. Psychol Med. 1993;23: 771–778. doi: 10.1017/s003329170002554x 8234583

18. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger. New York: Basic Books; 1973.

19. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Sadness and depression. New York: Basic Books; 1980.

20. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. 2nd ed. New York: Basic Books; 1982.

21. Birchwood M. Familial factors in psychiatry. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry. 1992;5: 295–299.

22. Birchwood M, Spencer E. Psychotherapies for schizophrenia: a review. In: Maj M, Sartorius N, editors. Schizophrenia. Chichester: Wiley; 1999. pp. 139–241.

23. Patterson P. Attachment, loss and expressed emotion: developmental processes in psychosis. In: Gumley A, Gillham A, Taylor K, Schwannauer M, editors. Psychosis and emotion: the role of emotions in understanding psychosis, therapy and recovery. London: Routledge; 2013. pp. 136–148.

24. Shaver PR, Fraley RC. Attachment, loss, and grief: Bowlby's views and current controversies. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment: theory, research, and clinical applications. New York: Guilford Press; 2008. pp. 48–77.

25. Shear K, Shair H. Attachment, loss, and complicated grief. Dev Psychobiol. 2005;47: 253–267. doi: 10.1002/dev.20091 16252293

26. Stroebe M, Schut H, Stroebe W. Attachment in coping with bereavement: a theoretical integration. Rev Gen Psychol. 2005;9: 48–66.

27. Fraley RC, Bonanno GA. Attachment and loss: a test of three competing models on the association between attachment-related avoidance and adaptation to bereavement. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. 2004;30: 878–90.

28. Neimeyer RA, Prigerson HG, Davies B. Mourning and meaning. Am Behav Sci. 2002;46: 235–251.

29. Bartholomew D, Horowitz LM. Attachment styles among young adults: A test of a four-category model. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991;61: 226–244. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.61.2.226 1920064

30. Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and change. New York: Guilford Press; 2007.

31. Burke LA, Neimeyer RA. Prospective risk factors for complicated grief: a review of the empirical literature. In: Stroebe MS, Schut H, van der Bout J, Boelen P, editors. Complicated grief. New York: Routledge; 2012. pp. 145–161.

32. Meier AM, Carr DR, Currier JM, Neimeyer RA. Attachment anxiety and avoidance in coping with bereavement: two studies. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2013;32: 315–34.

33. Godress J, Ozgul S, Owen C, Foley-Evans L. Grief experiences of parents whose children suffer from mental illness. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2005;39: 88–94. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1614.2005.01518.x 15660710

34. Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and change. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2016.

35. Cassidy J, Kobak RR. Avoidance and its relationship with other defensive processes. In: Belsky J, Nezworski T, editors. Clinical implications of attachment. Hillsdale: Erlbaum; 1988. pp. 300–323.

36. Main M. Cross-cultural studies of attachment organization: recent studies, changing methodologies, and the concept of conditional strategies. Hum Dev. 1990;33: 48–61.

37. Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. Adult attachment and affect regulation. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment: theory, research, and clinical applications, 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2008. pp. 503–531.

38. Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. Attachment insecurities and disordered patterns of grief. In: Stroebe M, Schut H, Boelen P, van den Bout J, editors. Complicated grief: scientific foundations for health care professionals. New York: Routledge; 2012. pp. 190–203.

39. Collins NL, Ford MB. Responding to the needs of others: the caregiving behavioral system in intimate relationships. J Soc Pers Relat. 2010;27: 235–244.

40. George C, Solomon J. The caregiving system: a behavioral systems approach to parenting. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment: theory, research, and clinical applications. New York: Guilford Press; 2008. pp. 833–857.

41. Jones JD, Cassidy J, Shaver PR. Parents’ self-reported attachment styles: a review of links with parenting behaviors, emotions, and cognitions. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2015;19: 44–76.

42. Kunce LJ, Shaver PR. An attachment theoretical approach to caregiving. In: Bartholomew K, Perlman D, editors. Attachment processes in adulthood. Advances in personal relationships. London: Jessica Kingsley; 1994. pp. 205–237.

43. Canterberry M, Gillath O. Attachment and caregiving functions, interactions, and implications. In: Noller P, Karantzas GC, editors. The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of couples and family relationships. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell; 2012. pp. 207–219.

44. Feeney BC, Collins NL. Predictors of caregiving in adult intimate relationships: an attachment theoretical perspective. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;80: 972–994. 11414378

45. Karantzas GC, Simpson JA. Attachment and aged care. In: Simpson JA, Rholes WS, editors. Attachment theory and research: new directions and emerging themes. New York: Guilford; 2012. pp. 319–345.

46. Chen CK. The relationship between attachment quality and expressed emotion among adult children caring for parents with dementia. Thesis, The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. 2006. Available from: https://cdr.lib.unc.edu/indexablecontent/uuid:ef702a96-6796-4067-9960-06057adb5292

47. Diamond D, Doane JA. Disturbed attachment and negative affective style: an intergenerational spiral. Br J Psychiatry. 1994;164: 770–781. doi: 10.1192/bjp.164.6.770 7952983

48. Paley G, Shapiro DA, Worrall-Davies A. Familial origins of expressed emotion in relatives of people with schizophrenia. J Ment Health. 2000;9: 655–663.

49. Adam EK, Gunnar MR, Tanaka A. Adult attachment, parent Emotion, and observed parenting behavior: mediator and moderator models. Child Dev. 2004;75: 110–122. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00657.x 15015678

50. Bosquet M, Egeland B. Associations among maternal depressive symptomatology, state of mind and parent and child behaviors: implications for attachment-based interventions. Attach Hum Dev. 2001;3: 173–199. doi: 10.1080/14616730010058007 11708736

51. García-Ruiz M, Rodrigo MJ, Hernández-Cabrera JA, Máiquez ML. Contribution of parents’ adult attachment and separation attitudes to parent-adolescent conflict resolution. Scand J Psychol. 2013;54: 459–67. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12077 24117437

52. Millings A, Walsh J, Hepper E, O’Brien M. Good partner, good parent. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. 2013;39: 170–180.

53. Berry K, Barrowclough C, Wearden A. A review of the role of adult attachment style in psychosis: unexplored issues and questions for further research. Clin Psychol Rev. 2007;27: 458–475. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.006 17258365

54. Domínguez-Martínez T, Vainer E, Antonia Massanet M, Torices I, Jané M, Barrantes-Vidal N. The need-adapted integrated treatment in Sant Pere Claver-Early Psychosis Program (SPC-EPP) in Barcelona, Spain. Salud Ment. 2011;34: 517–24.

55. Yung AR, Yuen HP, McGorry PD, Phillips LJ, Kelly D, Dell'Olio M, et al. Mapping the onset of psychosis: the comprehensive assessment of at-risk mental states. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2005;39: 964–971. doi: 10.1080/j.1440-1614.2005.01714.x 16343296

56. Schultze-Lutter F, Addington J, Ruhrmann S, Klosterkötter J. Schizophrenia proneness instrument–adult version (SPI-A). Rome: Giovanni Fioriti; 2007.

57. American Psychiatric Association, 2000. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000.

58. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JB. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders-Patient ed. (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0). New York: Biometrics Research Department; 1995.

59. Berry K, Wearden A, Barrowclough C, Liversidge T. Attachment styles, interpersonal relationships and psychotic phenomena in a non-clinical student sample. Pers Individ Dif. 2006;41: 707–718.

60. Wiedemann G, Rayki O, Feinstein E, Hahlweg K. The Family Questionnaire: Development and validation of a new self-report scale for assessing expressed emotion. 2002;109: 265–279.

61. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull. 1992; 112: 155–159. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.112.1.155 19565683

62. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press; 2013.

63. Collins NL, Feeney BC. A safe haven: An attachment theory perspective on support seeking and caregiving in intimate relationships. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;78: 1053–1073. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.6.1053 10870908

64. Álvarez-Jiménez M, Gleeson JF, Cotton SM, Wade D, Crisp K, Yap MBH, et al. Differential predictors of critical comments and emotional over-involvement in first-episode psychosis. Psychol Med. 2010;40: 63–72. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004765 19079825

65. Wynne LC. Current concepts about schizophrenics and family relationships. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1981;169: 82–89. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198102000-00003 7009785

66. O’Connell Corcoran K, Mallinckrodt B. Adult attachment, self-efficacy, perspective talking, and conflict resolution. J Couns Dev. 2000;78: 474–483.

67. Simpson JA, Rholes WS, Phillips D. Conflict in close relationships: an attachment perspective. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1996;71: 899–914. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.71.5.899 8939040

68. Feeney AJ. Parental attachment and conflict behavior: Implications for offspring’s attachment, loneliness, and relationship satisfaction. Pers Relatsh. 2006;13: 19–36.

69. Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. An attachment perspective on bereavement. In: Stroebe M, Hansson RO, Stroebe W, editors. Handbook of bereavement research and practice: 21st century perspectives. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2008. pp. 87–110.

70. Currier JM, Irish JEF, Neimeyer RA, Foster JD. Attachment, continuing bonds, and complicated grief following violent loss: testing a moderated model. Death Stud. 2015;39: 201–10. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2014.975869 25551174

71. Jerga AM, Shaver PR, Wilkinson RB. Attachment insecurities and identification of at-risk individuals following the death of a loved one. J Soc Pers Relat. 2011;28: 891–914.

72. Wijngaards-De Meij L, Stroebe M, Schut H, Stroebe W, Van Den Bout J, Van Der Heijden PGM, et al. Patterns of attachment and parents’ adjustment to the death of their child. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. 2007;33: 537–548.

73. Bowlby J. A secure base: parent-child attachment and healthy human development. New York: Basic Books; 1988.

74. Miklowitz DJ. The role of family systems in severe and recurrent psychiatric disorders: A developmental psychopathology view. 2004;16: 667–688. 15605631

75. Koutra K, Triliva S, Roumeliotaki T, Basta M, Simos P, Lionis C, et al. Impaired family functioning in psychosis and its relevance to relapse: a two-year follow-up study. Compr Psychiatry 2015;62: 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.06.006 26343461

76. Zanetti ACG, Ciol MA, Wiedemann G, de Azevedo-Marques JM, Dantas RAS, da Silva Gherardi-Donato EC, et al. Predictive Validity of the Brazilian Portuguese Version of the Family Questionnaire for Relapse in Patients With Schizophrenia. J Nurs Meas. 2017;25: 559–572. doi: 10.1891/1061-3749.25.3.559 29268835

77. Crisp K, Gleeson J. Working with families to prevent relapse in first episode psychosis. In: Lobban F, Barrowclough C, editors. A casebook of family interventions for psychosis. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons; 2009. pp. 67–90.

78. Lobban F, Postlethwaite A, Glentworth D, Pinfold V, Wainwright L, Dunn G, et al. A systematic review of randomised controlled trials of interventions reporting outcomes for relatives of people with psychosis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2013;33: 372–382. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.12.004 23410719

79. Harris T. Implications of attachment theory for working in psychoanalytic psychotherapy. Int Forum Psychoanal. 2004;13: 147–56.


Článok vyšiel v časopise

PLOS One


2019 Číslo 10
Najčítanejšie tento týždeň
Najčítanejšie v tomto čísle
Kurzy

Zvýšte si kvalifikáciu online z pohodlia domova

Získaná hemofilie - Povědomí o nemoci a její diagnostika
nový kurz

Eozinofilní granulomatóza s polyangiitidou
Autori: doc. MUDr. Martina Doubková, Ph.D.

Všetky kurzy
Prihlásenie
Zabudnuté heslo

Zadajte e-mailovú adresu, s ktorou ste vytvárali účet. Budú Vám na ňu zasielané informácie k nastaveniu nového hesla.

Prihlásenie

Nemáte účet?  Registrujte sa

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#