#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

Animal-assisted therapy for patients in a minimally conscious state: A randomized two treatment multi-period crossover trial


Authors: Karin Hediger aff001;  Milena Petignat aff001;  Rahel Marti aff001;  Margret Hund-Georgiadis aff002
Authors place of work: Department of Psychology, Division of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland aff001;  REHAB Basel, Clinic for neurorehabilitation and paraplegiology, Basel, Switzerland aff002;  Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, Human and Animal Health Unit, Swiss Tropical and Public Health Institute, Basel, Switzerland aff003;  Institute for Interdisciplinary Research on the Human-Animal Relationship Switzerland, Basel, Switzerland aff004
Published in the journal: PLoS ONE 14(10)
Category: Research Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0222846

Summary

Objective

To investigate if animal-assisted therapy (AAT) leads to higher consciousness in patients in a minimally conscious state during a therapy session, measured via behavioral reactions, heart rate and heart rate variability.

Methods

In a randomized two treatment multi-period crossover trial, 10 patients in a minimally conscious state participated in eight AAT sessions and eight paralleled conventional therapy sessions, leading to 78 AAT and 73 analyzed control sessions. Patients’ responses during sessions were assessed via behavioral video coding and the Basler Vegetative State Assessment (BAVESTA), heart rate and heart rate variability (SDNN, RMSSD, HF and LF). Data were analyzed with generalized linear mixed models.

Results

Patients showed more eye movements (IRR = 1.31, 95% CI: 1.23 to 1.40, p < 0.001) and active movements per tactile input during AAT compared to control sessions (IRR = 1.13, 95% CI: 1.02 to 1.25, p = 0.018). No difference was found for positive emotions. With BAVESTA, patients’ overall behavioral reactions were rated higher during AAT (b = 0.11, 95% CI: 0.01 to 0.22, p = 0.038). AAT led to significantly higher LF (b = 5.82, 95% CI: 0.55 to 11.08, p = 0.031) and lower HF (b = -5.80, 95% CI: -11.06 to -0.57, p = 0.030), while heart rate, SDNN, RMSSD did not differ.

Conclusions

Patients in a minimally conscious state showed more behavioral reactions and increased physiological arousal during AAT compared to control sessions. This might indicate increased consciousness during therapeutic sessions in the presence of an animal.

Trial registration

ClinicalTrials.gov NCT02629302.

Keywords:

Dogs – Behavior – Animal behavior – emotions – Consciousness – heart rate – traumatic brain injury – Eye movements

Introduction

Acquired brain injuries can result in severe disorders of consciousness, such as minimally conscious state (MCS), with often serious lifelong consequences for patients and their families [13]. Early onset of rehabilitation is a crucial factor with the goal of enhancing the patient’s consciousness by creating learning possibilities [4,5]. Current treatment concepts focus on stimuli which are activity-oriented and relevant for the individual patients, because personally and emotionally relevant stimuli induce higher-level activation in patients with disorders of consciousness [6,7]. Since animals are highly emotionally relevant [8], animal-assisted therapy (AAT) is an increasingly utilized approach in neurorehabilitation. AAT is a goal-directed intervention, in which a trained animal is an integral part of therapeutic activities [9]. Although there is anecdotal practical evidence [10], and AAT is becoming increasingly common in treatment of disorders of consciousness, empirical evidence from randomized controlled studies is lacking. Therefore, the aim of this study was to examine the effect of AAT on consciousness in patients in a minimally conscious state compared to conventional standard therapy in a randomized controlled trial. To investigate effects on patients’ consciousness, we assessed patients’ behavior and measured physiological arousal via heart rate and heart rate variability.

Methods

Participants

Subjects were 10 inpatients in a minimally conscious state. All patients were in stationary neurorehabilitation in a Swiss rehabilitation clinic, aged 17 to 71 years (M = 47.20, SD = 19.36) and diagnosed with acquired brain injury with either traumatic (N = 4) or non-traumatic causes (N = 6). Secondary diagnoses were not considered. Severity of the disorder of consciousness was assessed via the original JFK Coma Recovery Scale (CRS) [11,12] that does not include all of the behavioral criteria necessary to diagnose the minimally conscious state. The diagnosis was therefore based on clinical assessment by the responsible physician according to the Aspen diagnostic criteria [13] and to Bruno and colleagues [14] for the division of MCS+ and MCS-. MCS+ is characterized by the presence of command following, intelligible verbalization or gestural or verbal yes/no responses. MCS- patients in contrast only show minimal levels of behavioral interaction characterized by the presence of non-reflex movements. Patients were eligible for participation in the study if the scores and clinical assessment indicated a minimally conscious state following an acquired brain injury. Exclusion criteria were medical contraindications, such as phobias and allergies, assessed via interviews with relatives. The data was collected from May 2015 until April 2017. Fig 1 shows the CONSORT flowchart.

CONSORT flowchart.
Fig. 1. CONSORT flowchart.

Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and participant consents

The screening process involved the family members of the patients as well as the responsible physicians and therapists. The legal representative of the patients provided written informed consent. The human-related protocols were approved by the Ethics Committee for Northwest and Central Switzerland and the animal-related protocols were approved by the Veterinary Office of the Canton Basel-Stadt, Switzerland. AAT was performed according to the guidelines of the International Association of Human Animal Interaction Organizations (IAHAIO) to ensure patient safety and animal welfare [9]. No therapy session had to be ended early and no adverse incidents occurred. After participating in the study, all patients had the possibility to continue with AAT. The study was registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (Identifier: NCT02629302).

Study design and procedure

The study was designed as a randomized two treatment multi-period crossover design to evaluate the immediate effects of the different interventions on patient reactions. Standardized therapy sessions that integrated an animal served as experimental condition and are referred to as AAT sessions. In the control condition, paralleled, comparable standardized therapy sessions without the presence of an animal (treatment as usual) were used. Each patient participated in 16 therapy sessions over a period of 4 weeks (N AAT = 8, N control = 8). Sessions lasted for approximately 15 minutes and were held four times a week, twice with an animal and twice without an animal. Each control session was paralleled with an AAT session such that two sessions in two consecutive weeks were as similar as possible regarding the involved therapist, day of the week, time of day and therapeutic activity. All participants were allocated randomly to start with either AAT or a control session. Allocation sequence was generated via a random number generator by the principal investigator who also enrolled and assigned participants to interventions. Some of the originally planned 160 sessions were cancelled due to illness of patient or therapist, and for some sessions data was lost due to technical problems. In total, we coded the behavior of 151 sessions (N AAT = 78, N control = 73) and analyzed assessment data of 136 sessions (N AAT = 69, N control = 67) and heart rate data of 115 sessions (N AAT = 61, N control = 54). All AAT and control sessions were held in a therapy room within the therapy animal facility at the rehabilitation center. The patients were transported to the therapy room by wheelchair. Patients wore a heart rate monitor belt on their chest which continuously measured heart rate and heart rate variability during the session. All sessions were videotaped and at the end of each session, the behavior of the patients was assessed via the Basler Vegetative State Assessment by the therapists. Prior to the study start, a suitable animal was selected for each patient according to preference and abilities. Included species were dogs, guinea pigs and rabbits. All animals were trained for AAT, had experience working with patients in a minimally conscious state, and were kept and handled according to the IAHAIO standards [9]. Guinea pigs and rabbits were put into a table cage where they could interact with patients or retreat at will. During the AAT sessions, therapeutic activities were performed by physically guiding the patient’s hands according to the Affolter concept [15]. Examples of therapeutic activities were: brushing a dog, cutting vegetables and feeding them to the rabbits or guinea pigs, or opening a box with herbs and feeding them to the rabbits or guinea pigs. Paralleled control sessions consisted of therapeutic interventions with basic activities selected from a range of occupational therapy assignments. These activities were also performed according to the Affolter concept. Corresponding examples of control activities were: brushing a fake fur, preparing food by cutting vegetables and putting them in a bowl, or opening an empty box and filling it.

Behavioral analysis via video coding

As primary outcome, the patient’s reactions was assessed via behavioral video coding. Therapy sessions (N = 151) were videotaped with a handheld camera (Sony HDR-CX240) and analyzed with a behavioral coding system software (Observer XT 12, Noldus). Analyses were done continuously, defining each second of the video with the different variables as present or not for state behavior variables. We calculated the percentage of the duration of each state variable in relation to the observed time period of a therapy session. Count variables were coded only if they occurred, and the total occurrence within a therapy session was calculated. All videos were coded according to a strict ethogram defined by detailed descriptions of the behaviors with inclusion and exclusion examples. The coding scheme was developed for the purpose of this study. As basis, 11 existing paper-pencil behavioral assessment tools in German and English for patients with disorders of consciousness were screened. Items were pooled and reduced to behaviors that could be observed during video analysis and that occur according to a stimulus during a therapeutic situation. Our coding scheme included the dimensions “eyes open/closed”, “eye movement”, “movement”, “phonation” and “emotion” (operationalized via facial expression). Moreover, we coded the amount of verbal and tactile stimuli offered by the therapist as well as the amount of the patient’s physical contact with the animal. Inter-rater reliability was measured by Cohen’s kappa for all coded variables. Before coding the actual data, each rater achieved an inter-rater reliability of k > 0.80. Inter-rater reliability ranged between 0.83 and 0.99 indicating excellent agreement among coders.

Basler Vegetative State Assessment

The Basler Vegetative State Assessment (BAVESTA) [16], a behavioral assessment tool for patients with disorders of consciousness, was used as an additional tool to measure behavioral reactions of patients during each therapy session and served as secondary outcome. This study used 22 of the original 33 items, targeting behaviors that are observable during a short period of time, and adjusted the calculations of total short-term mean score and short-term subscores accordingly, with a range from 0 (behavior is not shown) to 5 (behavior is consistently shown). After each therapy session, the therapist assessed the patient with this short-term BAVESTA.

Heart rate and heart rate variability recording

Heart rate (HR) and heart rate variability (HRV) were measured using non-invasive HR monitoring belts (Polar® RS800CX, Polar® Electro Oy) as further secondary outcomes. The recorded inter-beat intervals were analyzed with Kubios HRV analysis software version 3.0.2 (Biosignal Analysis and Medical Imaging Group, University of Kuopio, Finland). In each therapy session, a 5-minute recording was selected. In control sessions, the 5-minute sequence was taken from the middle of the whole session. For AAT sessions, the duration of interaction between the patient and the animal was identified via the videos and the 5-minute sequence was taken from the middle of the interaction phase. Before processing, all RR-series were visually checked and, when necessary, artifacts were corrected. If the number of corrected beats was higher than 5%, the data was excluded from analysis (N AAT = 1, N control = 2). We also excluded data if the total recording or the interaction between the patient and the animal was shorter than 5 minutes (N = 1). We calculated the following HRV parameters: time domain: the standard deviation of all normal-to-normal RR intervals (SDNN, ms) and root-mean square differences of successive RR intervals (RMSSD, ms); and frequency domain: relative power of the low frequency (LF) and high frequency (HF) band in normal units.

Statistical analysis

Behavior analysis was performed using generalized linear mixed models. Count data were modeled as rates using a Poisson distribution and the logarithm of the duration of the sessions as an offset variable. The primary models included only the outcome variable and the treatment type as single predictor. Participant IDs were included as random effect to account for multiple observations within each subject. The Incident Rate Ratio (IRR) was used as effect size. The model holds under the assumption that there is no time effect which might be violated. Therefore, we checked the robustness of the model by fitting a second model equivalent to the previous one but including session number as a categorical fixed effect. During data inspection we noticed that the therapists behave differently in AAT and control sessions, primarily with respect to the number of tactile inputs. Because those inputs trigger most of the patients’ reactions, we fitted a third model that includes time as well as the log of tactile inputs. For descriptive statistics, the number of observed count behaviors (count variables) was transformed into rate per time ((n/time)*100 sec) and rate per tactile inputs ((n/tactile inputs)*100). To analyze the effect of AAT on BAVESTA scores and HR/HRV parameters as secondary outcomes, generalized linear mixed models with condition as fixed effect and the individual patient as random effect with the mean difference (b) as effect size was used. All variables were visually checked to detect extreme values (histogram and Q-Q-plot). Model diagnostics of linear mixed models included visual checks for normality of residuals and homogeneity of residuals. All residuals were approximately normally distributed with the exception of RMSSD, which was therefore log-normal transformed. No data were excluded except for HR/HRV data with corrected beats greater than 5% and recordings where patient and animal interacted for less than 5 minutes. Sample size was estimated based on clinical experience and on a pre-analysis of an ongoing study. The significance level was set at the 5% level and all statistical analyses were performed using SPSS, Version 24, and R, Version 3.5.1.

Results

Two female and eight male participants, between age 17 to 71 with an average age of 47 years (M = 47.20, SD = 19.36), participated in this study. CRS values at study start ranged between 14 and 22. All patients were diagnosed with MCS in a clinical assessment by the responsible physician according to the Aspen diagnostic criteria [13] and the criteria of Bruno and colleagues [14]. Table 1 summarizes the principal clinical and demographic characteristics of participants. Table 2, S1 and S2 Tables provide an overview of the intervention characteristics.

Tab. 1. Sample characteristics.
Sample characteristics.
Tab. 2. Intervention characteristics.
Intervention characteristics.

Behavior analysis

There were more tactile inputs from therapists during control sessions than during AAT sessions (control: M = 148.04, SD = 71.51, AAT: M = 114.19, SD = 57.35; IRR = 0.74, 95% CI: 0.68 to 0.81, p < 0.001), while verbal inputs from therapists did not differ significantly between conditions (control: M = 33.86, SD = 21.45, AAT: M = 28.66, SD = 18.86; b = -0.05, 95% CI: -0.11 to 0.01, p = 0.074).

Patients showed a significantly higher rate of eye movement of 5 movements per 100 seconds during AAT compared to control therapy sessions with a rate of 4 (IRR = 1.17, 95% CI: 1.11 to 1.24, p < 0.001). This effect was also present for the models that include time or time and tactile input. The rate of eye movement per tactile input even increased by a factor of 1.7 during AAT compared to control therapy sessions. The rate of total movements per 100 seconds was higher during control therapy sessions and decreased from 3.5 to 3 during AAT (IRR = 0.85, 95% CI: 0.80 to 0.91, p < 0.001). However, this effect reversed when time and tactile inputs were added to the model (IRR = 1.08, 95% CI: 1.00 to 1.16, p = 0.048) with a rate of 23 during AAT and a rate of 20 movements per 100 seconds during control therapy sessions. While there was no difference in self-initiated (active) movements in model one or two, patients showed significantly more self-initiated movements per tactile input during AAT compared to control therapy sessions (IRR = 1.13, 95% CI: 1.02 to 1.25, p = 0.018). The opposite effect was found for reactive movements of the patients. The rate of reactive movements per time was lower during AAT sessions (IRR = 0.74, 95% CI:0.67 to 0.80, p < 0.001) but this difference disappeared when looking at the rate per tactile input. Patients showed a higher amount of phonation during AAT compared to control therapy sessions (IRR = 1.92, 95% CI: 1.32 to 2.78, p < 0.001) but again, this effect disappeared when time and tactile inputs were included in the model. There was no difference regarding positive emotions, operationalized via positive facial expressions. Negative emotions were reduced during AAT compared to control therapy sessions but this difference was only statistically significant when the amount of tactile inputs were taken into account (IRR = 0.35, 95% CI: 0.20 to 0.59, p < 0.001, see Table 3).

Tab. 3. Analyzed behaviors during AAT and control therapy sessions.
Analyzed behaviors during AAT and control therapy sessions.

Basler Vegetative State Assessment

In the BAVESTA, the patients overall behavioral reactions were rated higher during AAT sessions compared to control sessions (b = 0.11, 95% CI: 0.01 to 0.22, p = 0.038). While there was no difference regarding the subscales “attention”, “verbal communication” “emotional reactions” or “motor reactions”, we found significantly higher perception and information processing scores (perception: b = 0.21, 95% CI: 0.01 to 0.41, p = 0.041; information processing: b = 0.19, 95% CI: 0.03 to 0.34, p = 0.023) as well as significantly more nonverbal communication (b = 0.19, 95% CI: 0.05 to 0.33, p = 0.010) during AAT compared to standard therapy sessions (see Table 4).

Tab. 4. Basler Vegetative State Assessment after AAT and control therapy sessions.
Basler Vegetative State Assessment after AAT and control therapy sessions.

Heart rate / heart rate variability

Heart rate as well as heart rate variability parameters SDNN and RMSSD did not differ significantly between AAT and control sessions (see Table 5). In contrast, patients showed significantly higher LF (b = 5.82, 95% CI: 0.55 to 11.08, p = 0.031) and lower HF values (b = -5.80, 95% CI: -11.06 to -0.57, p = 0.030) during AAT compared to control sessions.

Tab. 5. Heart rate and heart rate variability.
Heart rate and heart rate variability.

Discussion

We present the first randomized controlled trial of patients in a minimally conscious state assessing behavioral reactions and arousal during AAT and control therapy sessions. AAT led to significantly more eye movements, self-initiated movements as well as movements in total compared to control therapy sessions in the systematic behavior analysis. This is in line with results of Bardl and Bardl’s case-study [10] that documented improvements in visual exploration, spontaneous reactions and target-oriented movements in a patient in a persistent vegetative state during the presence of a dog, as well as Jones, Rice and Cottons’ review who showed increased engagement during therapy due to AAT in adolescents with mental health disorders [17]. We did not find differences in positive emotional reactions which somewhat contrasts to previously published results. In the BAVESTA, patients had a higher total score during AAT indicating higher consciousness, and they showed more nonverbal communication and higher perception and information processing scores. No verbal communication was shown by most of the patients, so it is not surprising that we found no difference. The BAVESTA subscales “attention”, “emotional reactions” and “motor reactions” did not differ between the conditions. The observed effects of higher behavior reactions in the presence of an animal, measured using two different approaches, indicate a higher level of awareness [18], one of the two components of consciousness [19]. While the previous study involved a dog [10], our study documents that guinea-pigs and rabbits might have same beneficial effects as dogs.

We observed differences in the frequency domain heart rate variability parameters. During AAT sessions, HF values were significantly lower and values of LF were significantly higher compared to conventional standard therapy. The decrease in HF values reflects decreased activity of the parasympathetic nervous system, while the increase in LF values is associated with increased activity of the sympathetic nervous system [20,21], so both outcomes indicate higher physiological activity and an increase in arousal in the presence of an animal. Along with awareness, arousal is the other component of consciousness [19]. Increased arousal could therefore reflect a higher level of consciousness and indicate an underlying process that might explain the observed behavioral effects of the patients in the presence of an animal. Lowered values of HF have also been associated with mental activity and mental stress [22,23]. But the observed HF in the AAT condition was within the range of normal values [20] and the reduction might also indicate an increase in arousal associated with positive emotions, excitement and emotional involvement [24,25] rather than distress. However, since patients in a minimally conscious state are highly vulnerable, further research is needed to clarify these effects. Our findings are in line with a previous investigation documenting lower values of HF in autistic children following interaction with a live dog compared to a robotic dog [26]. However, there are mixed outcomes from studies, documenting no effects [27] or even higher heart rate variability as a result of an interaction with an animal [28]. We found no statistically significant difference in heart rate between AAT and treatment as usual. This is in contrast to studies documenting decreases in heart rate during animal-assisted interventions for a broad range of populations [29] or an increased heart rate in hospitalized children with chronic disorders prior to and following dog assisted therapy as compared to control therapy sessions [30].

Neither participants nor raters responsible for video coding could be blinded to the conditions. The crossover design of this study only allowed for detecting short-term effects of AAT on behavioral and heart rate measurements during therapy sessions, and the small sample size limits the study outcomes and warrants further trials with more patients. Strengths of this study are inclusion of a paralleled control condition, behavior measured with different approaches and inclusion of a physiological parameter to identify underlying mechanisms. Moreover, our results showed that the presence of an animal can also influence the behavior of the involved therapists and that patients’ reactions should be interpreted in relation to the behavior of the therapists. This is a relevant aspect that should be taken into account in further study designs.

Conclusion

Our results indicate that AAT is a feasible approach to increase behavioral reactions and arousal in patients in a minimally conscious state. Integration of animals could be used to increase consciousness of these patients and lead to achieving a relevant therapeutic goal. Although this result is promising, the data are preliminary and it is necessary to further investigate whether AAT might be an effective approach to improve therapeutic effects of neurorehabilitation for patients in a minimally conscious state.

Supporting information

S1 Fig [tif]
Rate of eye movement during AAT and control therapy sessions over the course of the time.

S2 Fig [tif]
Rate of eye movement for each patient over the course of time.

S3 Fig [tif]
Rate of eye movement in the presence of different animals.

S4 Fig [tif]
Rate of total movement during AAT and control therapy sessions over the course of the time.

S5 Fig [tif]
Rate of total movement for each patient over the course of time.

S6 Fig [tif]
Rate of active movement during AAT and control therapy sessions over the course of the time.

S7 Fig [tif]
Rate of active movement for each patient over the course of time.

S8 Fig [tif]
Correlation matrix of the analyzed behaviors.

S1 Table [docx]
Sessions held by the different therapists.

S2 Table [docx]
Allocation of therapy sessions to different days of the week.

S3 Table [docx]
Correlations of the analyzed behaviors.

S1 File [doc]
CONSORT checklist.

S2 File [pdf]
Trial protocol.


Zdroje

1. Covelli V, Sattin D, Giovannetti AM, Scaratti C, Willems M, Leonardi M. Caregiver’s burden in disorders of consciousness: a longitudinal study. Acta Neurol Scand. 2016;134:352–359. doi: 10.1111/ane.12550 26748540

2. Pisa FE, Biasutti E, Drigo D, Barbone F. The prevalence of vegetative and minimally conscious states: a systematic review and methodological appraisal. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2014;29:E23–30. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0b013e3182a4469f 24052091

3. Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA, Bagnato S, Boccagni C, Galardi G. Life or death: prognostic value of a resting EEG with regards to survival in patients in vegetative and minimally conscious states. PLoS One. 2011;6(10):e25967. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0025967 21998732

4. La Gattuta E, Corallo F, Lo Buono V, De Salvo S, Cminiti F., Rifici C, et al. Techniques of cognitive rehabilitation in patients with disorders of consciousness: a systematic review. Neurol Sci. 2018;39:1–5.

5. Pistarini C, Maggioni G. Early rehabilitation of Disorders of Consciousness (DOC): management, neuropsychological evaluation and treatment. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2018;28:1319–1330. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2018.1500920 30033818

6. Perrin F, Castro M, Tillmann B, Luaute J. Promoting the use of personally relevant stimuli for investigating patients with disorders of consciousness. Front Psychol. 2015;6:1102. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01102 26284020

7. Sun Y, Wang J, Heine L, Huang W, Wang J, Hu N, et al. Personalized objects can optimize the diagnosis of EMCS in the assessment of functional object use in the CRS-R: a double blind, randomized clinical trial. BMC Neurol. 2018;18:38. doi: 10.1186/s12883-018-1040-5 29649978

8. Borgi M, Cirulli F. Pet Face: Mechanisms Underlying Human-Animal Relationships. Front Psychol. 2016;7:298. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00298 27014120

9. IAHAIO. IAHAIO White Paper 2014, updated for 2018. The IAHAIO definitions for animal-assisted intervention and guidelines for wellness of animals involved in AAI. http://iahaio.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2018/04/iahaio_wp_updated-2018-final.pdf2018. Accessed February 21, 2019.

10. Bardl S, Bardl M, Kornhuber ME. [Dog-based multi-sensorial therapy of a patient with a "persistent vegetative state"—a case report]. Rehabilitation. 2013;52:399–405. doi: 10.1055/s-0033-1334915 24217875

11. Giacino JT, Kezmarsky MA, DeLuca J, Cicerone KD. Monitoring rate of recovery to predict outcome in minimally responsive patients. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1991;72:897–901. doi: 10.1016/0003-9993(91)90008-7 1929808

12. Giacino JT, Kalmar K, Chase R. The JFK Coma Recovery Scale: Further Evidence for Applicability in Grading Level of Neurobehavioral Responsiveness Folowwing Severe Brain Injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1993;74:662.

13. Giacino JT, Ashwal S, Childs N, Cranford R, Jennett B, Katz DI, et al. The minimally conscious state: definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology. 2002;58:349–353. doi: 10.1212/wnl.58.3.349 11839831

14. Bruno MA, Vandenhause A, Thibaut A, Moonen G, Laureys S. From unresponsive wakefulness to minimally consious PLUS and functional locked-in syndromes: recent advances in our understanding of disorders of consiousness. J Neurol. 2011;258:1373–1384. doi: 10.1007/s00415-011-6114-x 21674197

15. Affolter F, Bischofberger W, Stockman IJ. Nonverbal perceptual and cognitive processes in children with language disorder. Toward a new framework for clinical intervention. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates 2000.

16. Huber M, Koch S, Hund-Georgiadis M, Mäder M, Borgwardt S. Stieglitz RD. Diagnostic Validity of the Basler Vegetative State Assessment—BAVESTA. International Journal of Health Professions. 2014;1:50–60.

17. Jones MG, Rice SM, Cotton SM. Incorporating animal-assisted therapy in mental health treatments for adolescents: A systematic review of canine assisted psychotherapy. PloS one. 2019; 14(1), e0210761. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0210761 30653587

18. Eapen BC, Georgekutty J, Subbaro B, Bavishi S, Cifu DX. Disorders of Consiousness. Phys Md Rehabil Clin N Am. 2017;28:245–258.

19. Laureys S. The neural correlate of (un)awareness: lessons from the vegetative state. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:556–559. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.010 16271507

20. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. Heart rate variability. Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Eur Heart J. 1996;17:354–381. 8737210

21. Thomas BL, Claassen N, Becker P, Viljoen M. Validity of Commonly Used Heart Rate Variability Markers of Autonomic Nervous System Function. Neuropsychobiology. 2019;5:1–13.

22. Hjortskov N, Rissen D, Blangsted AK, Fallentin N, Lundberg U, Søgaard K. The effect of mental stress on heart rate variability and blood pressure during computer work. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2004;92:84–89. doi: 10.1007/s00421-004-1055-z 14991326

23. Kim HG, Cheon EJ, Bai DS, Lee YH, Koo BH. Stress and Heart Rate Variability: A Meta-Analysis and Review of the Literature. Psychiatry Investig. 2018;15:235–245. doi: 10.30773/pi.2017.08.17 29486547

24. Kreibig SD. Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: a review. Biol Psychol. 2010;83:394–421.

25. Zupan M, Buskas J, Altimiras J, Keeling LJ. Assessing positive emotional states in dogs using heart rate and heart rate variability. Physiol Behav. 2016;155:102–111. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.11.027 26631546

26. Silva K, Lima M, Santos-Magalhaes A, Fafiaes C, de Sousa L. Can Dogs Assist Children with Severe Autism Spectrum Disorder in Complying with Challenging Demands? An Exploratory Experiment with a Live and a Robotic Dog. J Altern Complement Med. 2018;24:238–242.

27. Schretzmayer L, Kotrschal K, Beetz A. Minor Immediate Effects of a Dog on Children’s Reading Performance and Physiology. Front Vet Sci. 2017;4:90. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2017.00090 28674695

28. Motooka M, Koike H, Yokoyama T, Kennedy NL. Effect of dog-walking on autonomic nervous activity in senior citizens. Med J Aust. 2006;184:60–63. 16411869

29. Ein N, Li L, Vickers K. The effect of pet therapy on the physiological and subjective stress response: A meta-analysis. Stress Health. 2018; 34:477–489. doi: 10.1002/smi.2812 29882342

30. Kaminski M, Pellino T, Wish J. Play and pets: The physical and emotional impact of child-life and pet therapy on hospitalized children. Children’s Health Care. 2002;31:321–335.


Článok vyšiel v časopise

PLOS One


2019 Číslo 10
Najčítanejšie tento týždeň
Najčítanejšie v tomto čísle
Kurzy

Zvýšte si kvalifikáciu online z pohodlia domova

Získaná hemofilie - Povědomí o nemoci a její diagnostika
nový kurz

Eozinofilní granulomatóza s polyangiitidou
Autori: doc. MUDr. Martina Doubková, Ph.D.

Všetky kurzy
Prihlásenie
Zabudnuté heslo

Zadajte e-mailovú adresu, s ktorou ste vytvárali účet. Budú Vám na ňu zasielané informácie k nastaveniu nového hesla.

Prihlásenie

Nemáte účet?  Registrujte sa

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#